This week’s readings deal in
detail with the practice of conducting clinical interviews as a means of
assessing student thinking, as well as with the nature and purpose of various
forms of representation in math and science.
First, Ginsburg discusses at
length the set of practices and behaviors that, he believes, allow for success
in drawing out student thinking during clinical interviews. He makes a
distinction between the interviewer as researcher, who would like to examine
student thinking for the purpose of increasing/enhancing the general body of psychological
knowledge, and the interviewer as practitioner, who would like to examine
student thinking for the purpose of helping the student become a better
learner. Both roles rely heavily on a highly developed sensitivity for the
condition of the child in question (hence the term clinical), and both require a great deal of mental flexibility to
be able to provide the child with the necessary conditions for revealing her
mental processes to the interviewer. Ginsburg describes many potentially
helpful habits and dispositions, which may be applied before, during, or after
the interview. His recommendations, however, come with an ‘it depends’ caveat,
as Ginsburg points out that children are diverse and unique and will therefore
necessitate a diverse and unique repertoire of interview techniques.
Second, Russ and Sherin explore
the clinical interview from a teacher’s perspective, holding it up as an
effective and desirable way to assess student thinking prior to entering new units
of instruction. The utility of the clinical interview in the educator’s eyes,
according to the authors, lies in its ability to disclose to the teacher what
children already know, thereby
informing instruction and enabling the teacher to design curriculum that builds
on students’ existing ideas. The authors present three primary strategies in
using clinical interviews to explore student thinking: contextualize the
concept (give it real world ‘flesh and bones’), probe student responses (ask
how and why they got that answer, what they mean by x, etc.), and seed new ways of thinking (rephrasing questions,
giving hints, etc.).
Third, Greeno and Hall explore
practices of representation both in and out of school. They focus on
representation in math and science, though they are sure to point out that
forms of representation in other disciplines are fundamentally the same as in
science, despite their typically qualitative character. The authors present
representational practices not as an educational end in themselves, but rather
as a tool used by both students and professionals alike to assist in and give
form to the construction of new understandings, as well as to facilitate
communication of this understanding from person to person. A key idea from this
article is that a representation without someone there to interpret it is
simply notation, and as such lacks power as a tool for creation of new meaning.
Common threads:
- Students are active learners, capable of constructing complex understandings based on prior knowledge and current instruction;
- Student thinking can be articulated via original and/or canonical forms of representation;
- Student thinking should inform instruction, both through prior knowledge’s influence on curriculum and through the impact of students’ communicated understandings on other students.
I’m left thinking about how
clinical interviews ideally contain an opportunity for students to both engage
in modeling and to shape and communicate their understandings via
representation. The power of representations to be both foundations upon which
students can construct understandings as well as stages from which to pronounce
these understandings is an important asset to clinical interviewers and
educators. Not only does the content of representations aid the teacher in
assessing student learning, but also the language/form such representations
take. For example, many physical phenomena may be described visually (through
pictures), orally, mathematically, or graphically (through tables, charts,
etc.). The tendency of a student to employ a particular mode of representation,
or a combination of any, may surely be very telling to the teacher/interviewer
of how the student goes about describing to herself the nature of the world and
her learning about it. It is clear to me that the practices of representation, modeling,
explanation, and argumentation are very closely linked, and may be engaged in
the classroom through instruction, clinical interviews, and student
collaboration.
No comments:
Post a Comment
Note: Only a member of this blog may post a comment.