This
week’s readings centered on the concept of the model and its application to the
science classroom. In their
article on Modeling Instruction, Jackson et al make and argument for the
success of modeling and the correlated teacher training program in
physics. Jackson et al point to
modeling as a form of student-centered learning that most closely parallels the
construction and application of conceptual models used regularly in learning
and doing in professional research science. By inducing students to critically engage with their
experiences, develop independent lines of inquiry, and articulate their
findings, modeling is able to produce thoughtful students who more thoroughly
understand the underlying principles of physics.
The
two Lehrer and Schauble chapters look to consider the developmental roots and
applications of the modeling success that Jackon et al describe. In their chapter on the model, Lehrer
and Schauble discuss the disparity between the absence of modeling in the
classroom and the ubiquity of its use in scientific research. They present the reason for this as
two-fold- first that teachers misunderstand the meaning of experimentation and
use them as explanations instead of interactions, essentially scripting inquiry
to avoid ‘failure’. Second, they
describe modeling as an alien process for novices to understand, as children
may not have the cognitive structure to understand modeling relationships that
are not literal representations of the world. They devote the rest of the chapter to providing solutions
to these problems, pointing to ‘failure’ and ‘struggle’ as key opportunities
for students to learn and revise their misconceptions, and promoting step-wise
inquiry that builds from superficial models to more complex, abstract
forms.
In
their next chapter on experiment, Lehrer and Schauble provide more tangible
applications of modeling as experiment.
They argue that experimentation based in rhetoric, representation, and
modeling provides opportunity for more honest inquiry than exists in the
couched, predetermined experiments of contemporary classrooms. In their prescription of experimental
modeling, Lehrer and Schauble emphasize the importance of teacher scaffolding
as a support and guide for student’s independent, organic progression from
inquiry to experiment, allowing students to create their own logical
inscriptions and ultimately providing greater engagement with the
theories. Their examples point to
the success of argument over assertion resulting in comprehension over
superficial learning.
While
I absolutely believe in the importance of modeling in the classroom, it is hard
to ignore that both sets of authors used modeling as a sort of catch all term
for many other educational theories and best practices that were woven into the
presentation of modeling in the classroom. By providing variety of experiences, you appeal to multiple
learner types and by asking students to explain and defend their positions at
many stages, you are essentially using Socratic method to promote critical
thinking and providing the teacher with lots of opportunities to assess student
learning. Student motivation is
driven by ownership over their product and comprehension is aided by peer review
and support. What I’m trying to
get at is, could another method be as successful if taught so
thoughtfully?
Another
question I’m interested to discuss this week is Lehrer’s theory that modeling
is inherently unnatural for children.
In my experiences, I have found children to be naturally inquisitive and
creative, so what are the other necessary cognitive skills to model? Jackson et al point to metaphor as a
fundamental tool of human thought, can we not then appeal to the desire to
build connections and relationships that exists in humans of all ages? Lehrer and Schauble say that children
have ‘not necessarily mastered the strategies and heuristics for
experimentation that adults typically use’, are adult strategies necessarily the
best or should we look more to expound on the glorious ways kids already think?
First, is modeling considered any type of learning where the students are working hands on with some sort of materials? What would you call the other educational theories and best practices described in this week’s readings if they are not considered modeling? I’m curious to hear your opinions about what ways could students acquire knowledge that include ownership of products and peer review and support but would not be considered modeling in the classroom. Do you consider research or mathematics to be modeling? Lastly, do you think that adult strategies are best when designing experiments for young students? It was discussed sometime last week about how as students we have participated in similar if not identical science class experiments throughout primary and secondary school and undergraduate courses. Doesn’t this extinguish inquiry thought from students?
ReplyDeleteI see where you're coming from with regard to Lehrer's seemingly 'adults-only' presentation of some modeling techniques and the cognitive skills necessary to master them. I think that Lehrer was implying that much of modeling can require complex, abstract types of thinking that are commonly thought to be beyond children's developed abilities. I don't know if this is a natural cognitive development thing, where children deal strictly literally until they can deal partially abstractly, or if it perhaps has to do with some way in which children learn initially that encourages them to insist on literal similarity, but it seems to me that the remedy is simply more practice with modeling, teasing out specific characteristics and ignoring/controlling all others, so that students can grow accustomed to manipulating ideas and observations with those "fundamental tools of human thought."
ReplyDelete